A Comparison of Two Directed Monitoring Conditions for Improving Comprehension

Authors

  • Sandra Laing Gillam Emma Eccles Jones Early Childhood Education and Research Center, Utah State University, 2610 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322, USA
  • Jamison Fargo Emma Eccles Jones Early Childhood Education and Research Center, Utah State University, 2610 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322, USA
  • Ronald B. Gillam Emma Eccles Jones Early Childhood Education and Research Center, Utah State University, 2610 Old Main Hill, Logan, UT 84322, USA
  • Carrie Smith Cruce Greenville County Schools, USA

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.12970/2311-1917.2013.01.02.2

Keywords:

 Comprehension monitoring, comprehension, poor comprehends.

Abstract

Purpose: The purpose of the current study was to test the hypothesis that children, specifically poor comprehenders may demonstrate better comprehension monitoring and comprehension performance under conditions that 1) place emphasis on meaning rather than decoding, 2) do not impose undue demands on working memory and processing capacity, and 3) allow for more processing time.

Method: Fourth grade skilled (n = 20) and unskilled comprehenders (n = 20) listened to passages under two directed monitoring conditions. In one condition, children listened to entire passages (Listen Through; LT) and in the other condition children listened to passages one sentence at a time (Stop, Think and Monitor; STAMP). Participants were asked to listen for anomalous information, to report when and if they identified things in the passages that did not make sense, and to explain why they felt the information was erroneous. All children were asked to recall passages and answer explicit and implicit (inferential) questions about the passages after they listened to them.

Results: Skilled comprehenders performed better on identifying and resolving anomalous information under both directed monitoring conditions. While skilled comprehenders performed better on comprehension outcomes (recall, answering questions) in the LT condition, this was not true in the STAMP condition. Both groups performed comparably on comprehension outcomes, specifically in answering implicit questions in the STAMP condition. Partial correlation coefficients between the number of implicit questions answered correctly and detection and resolution of anomalies was not significant after controlling for the effects of language on the outcome variables.

Conclusions: Further research is needed to identify the most effective methods and procedures for teaching students to comprehend what they are hearing or reading. Underlying factors such as language ability and working memory may mediate the benefit received from an approach or strategy designed to improve comprehension performance. Our study suggests that the measure of comprehension chosen (recall or answering questions) to evaluate comprehension performance may dictate the type of intervention or strategy that is most effective.

References

Catts HW, Adolf SM, Ellis Weismer S. Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2006; 49: 278-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/023)

Hogan T, Bridges M, Justice L Cain K. Increasing higher level language skills to improve reading comprehension. Focus Except Child 2011; 44: 1-19.

Nation K, Clarke P, Marshall C, Durand M. Hidden language impairments in children: parallels between poor reading comprehension and specific language impairment? J Speech Lang Hear Res 2004; 47: 199-211. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/017)

Karasinski C, Ellis Weismer S. Comprehension of inferences in discourse processing by adolescents with and without language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2010; 53: 1268-79. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0006)

Oakhill J, Hartt J, Samols D. Levels of comprehension monitoring and working memory in good and poor comprehenders. Read Writ 2005; 18: 657-86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3355-z

Woodcock R. Woodcock reading mastery test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service 1987.

Dollaghan C. Comprehension monitoring in normal and language-impaired children. Top Lang Disord 1987; 7: 45-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00011363-198703000-00006

Walters DB, Chapman RS. Comprehension monitoring: A developmental effect? Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2000; 9: 48-54.

Skarakis-Doyle E, Mullin K. Comprehension monitoring in language-disordered children: A preliminary investigation of cognitive and linguistic factors. J Speech Hear Disord 1990; 55: 700-5.

Carr E, Dewitz P, Patberg J. Using cloze for inference training with expository text. Read Teach 1989; 42: 1-385.

Davoudi M. Inference generation skill and text comprehension. Reading Matrix 2005; 5: 106-23.

Oakhill J, Cain K, Bryant P. The dissociation of word reading and text comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Lang Cogn Proc 2003; 18: 443-68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960344000008

Yuill N, Oakhill J, Parkin A. Working memory, comprehension ability and the resolution of text anomaly. Br J Psychol 1989; 80: 351-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1989.tb02325.x

Baker L. 'Metacognition in comprehension instruction.' In: Block CC and Pressley M (Eds) Comprehension Instruction: Research-Based Best Practices. New York, NY: Guilford Press 2002; pp. 261-86.

Markman EM. Realizing that you don’t understand: A preliminary investigation. Child Dev 1977; 48: 986-92.

National Early Literacy Panel Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and its Implications for Reading Instruction. Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development 2010.

Richards JC Anderson NA. How do you know: a strategy to help emergent readers make inferences. Read Teach 2003; 57: 290-3.

Gersten R, Fuchs L, Williams J, Baker S. Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of research. Rev Educ Resh 2001; 71: 279-320. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00346543071002279

McMaster K, Kendeou P, Rapp DN, van den Broek P. Cognitive profiles of struggling, average, and good readers in elementary, middle, and high school. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading 2006; Vancouver, Canada.

Long D, Golding J. Superordinate goal inferences: are they automatically generated during comprehension? Discourse Process 1993; 16: 55-73. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638539309544829

Magliano J, Baggett W, Johnson B, Graesser A. The time course of generating causal antecedent and causal consequence inference. Discourse Process 1993; 16: 35-53. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01638539309544828

Windsor J, Milbrath R, Carney E, Rakowski S. General slowing in language impairment: methodological considerations in testing the hypothesis. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2001; 44: 446-61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/036)

Semel E, Wiig E, Secord W. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace 1995.

Brown L, Sherbenou R, Johnsen S. Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, Third Edition. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed 1998.

Gottardo A, Stanovich K, Siegel L. The relationships between phonological sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance of third-grade children. J Exp Child Psychol 1996; 63: 563-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0062

Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith S, O’Brien M. Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1997; 40: 1245-60.

Miller C, Kail, R, Leonard L,Tomblin B. Speed of processing in children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2001; 44: 416-33. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/034)

Ellis Weismer S, Hesketch L. Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1996; 39: 177-90.

Hoffman L, Gillam, R. Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2004; 47: 114-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/011)

Montgomery J, Windsor J. Examining the language performance of children with and without specific language impairment: contributions of phonological short-term memory and speed of processing. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2007; 50: 778-97. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/054)

McCroskey R, Thompson N. Comprehension of rate controlled speech by children with specific learning disabilities. J Learn Disabil 1973; 6: 621-7. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/002221947300601005

Berninger V, Abbott R, Swanson HL, et al. Relationship of word- and sentence –level working memory to reading and writing in second, fourth, and sixth grade. Lang Speech Hear Serv 2010; 41: 179-93. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2009/08-0002)

McNamara D, O’Reilly T. Theories of comprehension skill: knowledge and strategies versus capacity and suppression. Adv Psychol Res 2009; 62: 1-24.

Miller C, Leonard L, Kail R, Zhang X, Tomblin B, Francis D. Response time in 14-year-olds with language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2006; 49: 712-28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/052)

Miller J, Chapman R. Systematic analysis of language transcripts. Madison, WI: Language Analysis Laboratory 2004.

Weismer S, Hesketh L. Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1996; 39: 177-90.

Callahan S, Walenski M, Love T. The processing and interpretation of verb phrase ellipsis constructions by children at normal and slowed speech rates. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2012; 55: 710-25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0281)

Gillam S, Fargo J, Robertson K. Comprehension of expository text: Insights gained from think-aloud data. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2009; 18: 82-94. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2008/07-0074)

Laing S, Kamhi A. The use of think-aloud protocols to compare inferencing abilities in average and below-average readers. J Lear Disabil 2002; 35: 437-48. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00222194020350050401

Justice L, Gillam S, McGinty A. Teaching students with language disorders In R. McWilliam, M. Tankersley, B. Cook (Eds), Effective Practices in Special Education. New York, NY: Pearson 2012; pp. 61-72.

Long D, Golding J, Graesser A. A test of the on-line status of goal-related inferences. J Mem Lang 1992; 31: 634-47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(92)90032-S

Zabrucky K, Ratner H. Children’s comprehension monitoring and recall of inconsistent stories. Child Dev 1986; 57: 1401-18. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1130419

Downloads

Published

2013-06-02

Issue

Section

Articles