Predictive Bias Towards Neutral Stimuli in Non-Clinical Anxious Individuals

Authors

  • Pinchao Luo Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China
  • Wei Li Inner Mongolia Medical University , Hohhot 010000, P.R. China and Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot 010000, P.R. China
  • Xiaofeng Yang Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot 010000, P.R. China and Inner Mongolia University, Hohhot 010000, P.R. China
  • Heng Li School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China
  • Yu Pang School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China
  • Peiheng Feng School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China
  • Liang Xu School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China and Guangdong Communication Polytechnic, Guangzhou 510000, P.R. China
  • Qishisan Inner Mongolia Normal University, Hohhot 010000, P.R. China
  • Xifu Zheng Center for Studies of Psychological Application, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China and School of Psychology, South China Normal University, Guangzhou 510631, P.R. China

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.12970/2310-8231.2016.04.01.2

Keywords:

Non-clinical anxious individuals, predictable, unpredictable, predictive bias.

Abstract

The relationship among predictive bias towards neutral stimuli, trait anxiety and gender in non-clinical individuals is studied. According to the scores on Trait Anxiety Inventory (TAI), 31 individuals are randomly selected from the highest 20% scorers as the high anxiety group, and 31 individuals from the lowest 20% scorers as the low anxiety group. Three types of stimulus situations are designed in the experiment, that is, 100% predictable, 50% predictable and unpredictable stimulus situations. MANOVA, which is performed on the reaction of high/low anxiety groups under three stimulus situations, shows that significant differences exist between high anxiety and low anxiety group under the 50% predictable and unpredictable conditions. Independent sample T test shows significant gender differences on predictive bias exist only in high anxiety group. Results of this study show that predictive bias towards neutral stimulus can be found in non-clinical anxious individuals and is significantly correlated with trait anxiety. In addition, predictive bias is more evident in high anxious female.

References

Seligman ME. Chronic fear produced by unpredictable shock[J]. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1968; 66(2): 402-11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0026355

Weiss JM. Somatic Effects of Predictable and Unpredictable Shock[J]. Psychosomatic Medicine 1970; 32(4): 397-408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006842-197007000-00008

Grillon C, Baas J, Lissek SK, et al. Anxious responses to predictable and unpredictable aversive events[J]. Behavioral Neuroscience 2004; 118(5): 916-924. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.118.5.916

Grillon C. Models and mechanisms of anxiety: evidence from startle studies[J]. Psychopharmacology 2008; 199(3): 421-37. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-007-1019-1

Grillon C, Baas JM, Cornwell B, et al. Context conditioning and behavioral avoidance in a virtual reality environment: effect of predictability[J]. Biological Psychiatry 2006; 60(7): 752-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.03.072

Lejuez CW, Eifert GH, Zvolensky MJ, et al. Preference between onset predictable and unpredictable administrations of 20% carbon-dioxide-enriched air: implications for better understanding the etiology and treatment of panic disorder[J]. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 2000; 6(4): 349-58. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.6.4.349

Badia P, Suter S, Lewis P. Preference for warned shock: information and/or preparation [J]. Psychological Reports 1967; 20(1): 271-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1967.20.1.271

Lockard JS. Choice of a warning signal or none in several unavoidable-shock situations[J]. Psychonomic Science 1965; 3(1-12): 5-6.

Pervin LA. The need to predict and control under conditions of threat1[J]. Journal of Personality 1963; 31(4): 570-587. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1963.tb01320.x

Dan WG, Nitschke JB. Uncertainty and anticipation in anxiety: an integrated neurobiological and psychological perspective [J]. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2013; 14(7): 488-501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn3524

Kastner AK, Pauli P, Wieser MJ. Sustained attention in context conditioning: Evidence from steady-state VEPs[J]. International Journal of Psychophysiology 2015; 98(3): 546-556. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.03.005

Seligman MEP, Binik YM. The safety-signal hypothesis.In H. Davis & H. M. B. Hurwitz (Eds.), Operant-Pavlovian interactions [M], Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum 1977; pp. 165-187.

Cantor MB, Lolordo VM. Rats prefer signaled reinforcing brain stimulation to unsignaled ESB [J]. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1970; 71(71): 183-91. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0029122

Prokasy WF. The acquisition of observing responses in the absence of differential external reinforcement [J]. Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology 1956; 49(2): 131-4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0046740

Veening JG, Böcker KB, Verdouw PM, Olivier B, De Jongh R, Groenink L. Activation of the septohippocampal system differentiates anxiety from fear in startle paradigms [J]. Neuroscience 2009; 163: 1046-60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.06.064

Foa EB, Zinbarg R, Rothbaum BO. Uncontrollability and unpredictability in post-traumatic stress disorder: an animal model[J]. Psychological Bulletin 1992; 112(2): 218-38. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.218

Nelson BD, Mcgowan SK, Sarapas C, et al. Biomarkers of Threat and Reward Sensitivity Demonstrate Unique Associations With Risk for Psychopathology[J]. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2013; 122(122): 662-71. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033982

Shankman SA, Nelson BD, Sarapas C, et al. A psychophysiological investigation of threat and reward sensitivity in individuals with panic disorder and/or major depressive disorder[J]. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2013; 122(2): 322-338. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0030747

Craske MG, Glover D, Decola J. Predicted versus unpredicted panic attacks: acute versus general distress[J]. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1995; 104(104): 214-23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.104.1.214

Davis M, Walker DL, Grillon C. Phasic vs Sustained Fear in Rats and Humans: Role of the Extended Amygdala in Fear vs Anxiety[J]. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 35(1): 105-35. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.109

Davies CD, Craske MG. Psychophysiological responses to unpredictable threat: Effects of cue and temporal unpredictability[J]. Emotion 2015; 15(2): 195-200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000038

Felicia J, Nelson BD, Greg Hajcak P. In an uncertain world, errors are more aversive: evidence from the error-related negativity[J]. Emotion 2015; 15(1): 12-6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000020

Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, et al. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder[J]. Biological Psychiatry 2009; 66(12): 1075-82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026

Yulia G, Mauch CP, Maik D, et al. Consequences of extinction training on associative and non-associative fear in a mouse model of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD)[J]. Behavioural Brain Research 2009; 205(2): 544-9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2009.08.019

Downloads

Published

2016-03-06

Issue

Section

Articles